MIT study also pinpoints where the brain stores memory traces, both false and authentic.
The phenomenon of false memory has been well-documented: In many court
cases, defendants have been found guilty based on testimony from
witnesses and victims who were sure of their recollections, but DNA
evidence later overturned the conviction.
In a step toward
understanding how these faulty memories arise, MIT neuroscientists have
shown that they can plant false memories in the brains of mice. They
also found that many of the neurological traces of these memories are
identical in nature to those of authentic memories.
“Whether it’s
a false or genuine memory, the brain’s neural mechanism underlying the
recall of the memory is the same,” says Susumu Tonegawa, the Picower
Professor of Biology and Neuroscience and senior author of a paper
describing the findings in the July 25 edition of Science.
The
study also provides further evidence that memories are stored in
networks of neurons that form memory traces for each experience we have —
a phenomenon that Tonegawa’s lab first demonstrated last year.
Neuroscientists
have long sought the location of these memory traces, also called
engrams. In the pair of studies, Tonegawa and colleagues at MIT’s
Picower Institute for Learning and Memory showed that they could
identify the cells that make up part of an engram for a specific memory
and reactivate it using a technology called optogenetics.
Lead
authors of the paper are graduate student Steve Ramirez and research
scientist Xu Liu. Other authors are technical assistant Pei-Ann Lin,
research scientist Junghyup Suh, and postdocs Michele Pignatelli, Roger
Redondo and Tomas Ryan.
Seeking the engram
Episodic
memories — memories of experiences — are made of associations of
several elements, including objects, space and time. These associations
are encoded by chemical and physical changes in neurons, as well as by
modifications to the connections between the neurons.
Where these
engrams reside in the brain has been a longstanding question in
neuroscience. “Is the information spread out in various parts of the
brain, or is there a particular area of the brain in which this type of
memory is stored? This has been a very fundamental question,” Tonegawa
says.
In the 1940s, Canadian neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield
suggested that episodic memories are located in the brain’s temporal
lobe. When Penfield electrically stimulated cells in the temporal lobes
of patients who were about to undergo surgery to treat epileptic
seizures, the patients reported that specific memories popped into mind.
Later studies of the amnesiac patient known as “H.M.” confirmed that
the temporal lobe, including the area known as the hippocampus, is
critical for forming episodic memories.
However, these studies
did not prove that engrams are actually stored in the hippocampus,
Tonegawa says. To make that case, scientists needed to show that
activating specific groups of hippocampal cells is sufficient to produce
and recall memories.
To achieve that, Tonegawa’s lab turned to
optogenetics, a new technology that allows cells to be selectively
turned on or off using light.
For this pair of studies, the
researchers engineered mouse hippocampal cells to express the gene for
channelrhodopsin, a protein that activates neurons when stimulated by
light. They also modified the gene so that channelrhodopsin would be
produced whenever the c-fos gene, necessary for memory formation, was
turned on.
In last year’s study,
the researchers conditioned these mice to fear a particular chamber by
delivering a mild electric shock. As this memory was formed, the c-fos
gene was turned on, along with the engineered channelrhodopsin gene.
This way, cells encoding the memory trace were “labeled” with
light-sensitive proteins.
The next day, when the mice were put in
a different chamber they had never seen before, they behaved normally.
However, when the researchers delivered a pulse of light to the
hippocampus, stimulating the memory cells labeled with channelrhodopsin,
the mice froze in fear as the previous day’s memory was reactivated.
“Compared
to most studies that treat the brain as a black box while trying to
access it from the outside in, this is like we are trying to study the
brain from the inside out,” Liu says. “The technology we developed for
this study allows us to fine-dissect and even potentially tinker with
the memory process by directly controlling the brain cells.”
Incepting false memories
That
is exactly what the researchers did in the new study — exploring
whether they could use these reactivated engrams to plant false memories
in the mice’s brains.
First, the researchers placed the mice in
a novel chamber, A, but did not deliver any shocks. As the mice
explored this chamber, their memory cells were labeled with
channelrhodopsin. The next day, the mice were placed in a second, very
different chamber, B. After a while, the mice were given a mild foot
shock. At the same instant, the researchers used light to activate the
cells encoding the memory of chamber A.
On the third day, the
mice were placed back into chamber A, where they now froze in fear, even
though they had never been shocked there. A false memory had been
incepted: The mice feared the memory of chamber A because when the shock
was given in chamber B, they were reliving the memory of being in
chamber A.
Moreover, that false memory appeared to compete with a
genuine memory of chamber B, the researchers found. These mice also
froze when placed in chamber B, but not as much as mice that had
received a shock in chamber B without having the chamber A memory
activated.
The researchers then showed that immediately after
recall of the false memory, levels of neural activity were also elevated
in the amygdala, a fear center in the brain that receives memory
information from the hippocampus, just as they are when the mice recall a
genuine memory.
These two papers represent a major step forward
in memory research, says Howard Eichenbaum, a professor of psychology
and director of Boston University’s Center for Memory and Brain.
“They
identified a neural network associated with experience in an
environment, attached a fear association with it, then reactivated the
network to show that it supports memory expression. That, to me, shows
for the first time a true functional engram,” says Eichenbaum, who was
not part of the research team.
The MIT team is now planning further studies of how memories can be distorted in the brain.
“Now
that we can reactivate and change the contents of memories in the
brain, we can begin asking questions that were once the realm of
philosophy,” Ramirez says. “Are there multiple conditions that lead to
the formation of false memories? Can false memories for both pleasurable
and aversive events be artificially created? What about false memories
for more than just contexts — false memories for objects, food or other
mice? These are the once seemingly sci-fi questions that can now be
experimentally tackled in the lab.”
The research was funded by the RIKEN Brain Science Institute.
No comments:
Post a Comment